_C_O_N_S_T_R_A_I_N_T_S _O_N _C_H_A_N_G_E _I_N _C_H_I_L_D_R_E_N'_S _P_R_O_B_L_E_M-_S_O_L_V_I_N_G:
_T_H_E _R_O_L_E _O_F _S_P_E_C_I_F_I_C _S_T_R_A_T_E_G_I_E_S
Stephanie Thornton
CSRP 302, November 1993
Cognitive Science Research Paper
Serial No. CSRP 302
The 日韩无码
School of Cognitive and Computing Sciences
Falmer
BRIGHTON BN1 9QH
England, U.K.
2
_C_O_N_S_T_R_A_I_N_T_S _O_N _C_H_A_N_G_E _I_N _C_H_I_L_D_R_E_N'_S _P_R_O_B_L_E_M-_S_O_L_V_I_N_G:
_T_H_E _R_O_L_E _O_F _S_P_E_C_I_F_I_C _S_T_R_A_T_E_G_I_E_S
Stephanie Thornton
School of Cognitive and Computing Sciences
日韩无码
Brighton BN1 9QH, England, U.K.
E-mail: thorn@cogs.susx.ac.uk
November 1993
_A_b_s_t_r_a_c_t
This paper reports a study of the changes which children aged four to ten
years made to their problem-solving whilst playing the "Twenty Questions"
game. Factors specific to particular strategies are shown to play a central
role in triggering episodes of change, whilst developmental factors (prior
skill levels) determined the direction of the change. Analyses of these
effects are discussed in relation to the debate as to the relative
contributions of domain general and domain specific mechanisms in the
incidence of change in children's problem-solving.
1
Children change their strategies during the course of problem-solving.
Some of these changes are made in search of a solution to the problem.
Others have no clear connection to the demands of the task, and occur in
procedures already successfully solving the problem. Both types of effect,
and particularly the latter, have been interpreted as reflecting the
workings of developmental processes (e.g., Inhelder, Sinclair & Bovet, 1974;
Karmiloff-Smith, 1979, 1984; Klahr, 1984).
A key problem for developmental psychology is to identify the processes
which generate these phenomena of change. A number of theoretical models
have been put forward (e.g., Anderson, 1983; Klahr, 1982, 1984; Karmiloff-
Smith, 1984, 1992) However, there is no consensual view. Major issues
remain unresolved. For example, there is the problem of the relative
contributions of domain-neutral and domain-specific processes.
A domain-neutral process operates in all domains. It neither relies
on, nor directly uses the content of the domain for its functioning.
Processes of this kind are inevitably _s_y_n_t_a_c_t_i_c, in that they must act on
abstract structures or forms, without regard to the concrete content or
context of those structures. An example of such a syntactic domain-neutral
rule, simplified from an illustration given by Hunt (1989) would be: "If the
goal is to produce a structure of type Z, and the current knowledge state is
a structure of type Y, and if there is an operation that changes structures
of type Y into type Z, then attempt to transform the current knowledge
structure into a structure of type Z". This same rule applies across any
and all domains - so long as its conditions are met.
In contrast, a domain-specific process is particular to a given domain,
and irrelevant in all others. Processes of this kind are _s_e_m_a_n_t_i_c, in that
they operate directly through manipulations of representations of the
particular environment or domain in which they will be used. An example
(again, modified from Hunt 1989) of such a rule would be: "Always protect
2
the king first if you want to win at chess". Rules of this type will apply
only in particular contexts, just as the illustrative rule here applies only
to chess.
The majority of studies of mechanisms of cognitive change and
development have focussed on the role of domain-neutral syntactic factors.
For example, syntactic principles underly virtually all computational models
(cf. Anderson, 1983, Klahr, 1984). Syntactic mechanisms are also the basic
element in Karmiloff-Smith's (1992) representational redescriptive or RR
processes (where the same RR mechanism operates domain-neutrally, although
its products may be domain specific). These theories emphasize the role of
meta-processes in producing change to problem-solving procedures, by
monitoring ongoing problem-solving, identifying opportunities for the
execution of some syntactic rule of the kind illustrated above, and
executing it - the goals and criteria for such rules being predetermined,
and common across all domains.
However, it is almost certain that cognitive change also involves
semantic, domain-specific processes (cf. Carey, 1985; Keil, 1984, 1986).
Both domain-general and domain-neutral mechanisms have been shown to
contribute importantly to problem-solving at large (see Hunt, 1989 for a
review). Few developmentalists would want to argue that developmental
processes differ from cognition at large in this respect. But there has
been little research into the part played by domain-specific processes in
development or cognitive change. Domain-specific factors have been explored
almost exclusively in the context of problem-solving in the strict sense -
i.e., processes searching for the solution to a problem. There is almost no
work exploring how domain-specific processes might generate change in
procedures already successful in completing the task. Notable exceptions to
this are the work of Siegler and Jenkins (1989) on the generation of new
strategies, and by Wales and Thornton (1991) on conceptual change in
3
problem-solving. Both of these lines of work emphasise the role of the
semantic content of the initial strategy in mediating transitions to new
strategies and representations.
Both studies of domain-neutral mechanisms, and studies of domain-
specific ones have yielded impressive evidence in support of the activity of
each kind of mechanism in generating within-session change in problem-
solving. However, research in one vein is largely independent of that in the
other. There are no explorations of the relative contribution of each kind
of process, or of the interaction between the two. The aim of this paper is
to present a study addressing these issues.
In order to examine the relative role of domain-neutral and domain-
specific processes, it is essential to identify an empirical arena in which
each type of process generates predictions, and where these predictions
differ. This poses some problems. The data normally used in exploring
domain-neutral processes (e.g., demonstrations of very similar effects
across a range of situations) are very different from those used to explore
domain-specific ones (detailed analyzes of individual episodes of change).
Thus the normal types of prediction from each type of model are at very
different levels of analysis. A worse problem is that, for most episodes of
change, it is possible to generate either a domain-neutral or a domain-
specific explanation: neither approach has been specified in sufficient
detail to allow the identification of any critical differentiating
characteristic of change contributed by one type of mechanism or the other.
The approach taken here is to move away from examining the form change
takes, and to look in more detail at the incidence of change. A
consideration of the generic form of domain-neutral and domain-specific
models reveals that both predict that the incidence of change will be
differentially associated with different strategies - but for different
reasons.
4
In the case of domain-neutral models, the content of the strategy in
which these processes operate is irrelevant to their action. Nonetheless,
specific strategies will still have an effect on the occurrence of change:
syntactic processes will only act to generate change if their enabling
conditions (e.g., knowledge structure of type X) are met. Strategies are
likely to differ in the extent to which they generate the appropriate
enabling conditions for syntactic rules to operate. For instance, a
syntactic process for cutting out redundant steps in a procedure can only
operate in a strategy which involves redundant steps. Strategies more often
generating the enabling conditions for syntactic processes will be more
associated with change than those less often yielding these enabling
conditions.
In the case of domain-specific models, the content of a strategy is
more straightforwardly and obviously associated with the incidence of
change. For processes of this type, the incidence of change is directly
predicated on the interaction between semantic rules and the content of
ongoing processing: where ongoing processing meets the rule, change will not
be triggered. But where ongoing processing violates the semantic rule,
change will be triggered. Where strategies differ in the extent to which
they satisfy semantic rules pertinent to the domain in question (as must
often be the case), they will differ in their tendency to trigger change.
The relationship between specific strategies and change has been
explored qualitatively (Siegler & Jenkins, 1989; Wales & Thornton, 1991).
But there is no quantitative test of the general prediction that change is
more associated with some strategies than with others. Confirmation of the
occurrence of "strategy" effects in the incidence of change would open up a
new field of empirical exploration of the constraints on cognitive change.
For the purposes of the present paper, analyzes of the origin of such
"strategy" effects would provide an arena in which to study the relative
5
role of domain-neutral and domain-specific processes.
The aim of this study was therefore first to test the general
prediction that change in children's problem-solving is associated with some
strategies rather than with others; and second, to explore the contribution
of either semantic or syntactic factors to this effect.
Inevitably, a study of the contribution of specific strategies to
change in problem-solving requires very detailed analysis of concrete
situations. The analysis presented here relates to one task, the Twenty
Questions task. This task has several advantages. First, it has been
reported to generate a variety of strategies, and a great deal of within-
session change between strategies (Thornton, 1982). In the task as used by
Thornton all observed strategies successfully solve the problem, and the
task does not constrain the child to become more efficient, quicker, etc.
Thus the changes to problem-solving observed in this task involve change in
adequate procedures as well as problem-solving in search of a solution.
Furthermore, a percentage of the observed within-session change reported by
Thornton parallels the classically reported age effect in this task: the
transition from asking about items one by one to asking about a group of
items with a single question (cf. Mosher & Hornsby, 1966). The task
therefore allows scope for a comparison of a given effect as it occurs
within session and with increasing age. The experiment below replicated
Thornton's (1982) methodology, but with a wider age range of subjects.
Experiment 1
_M_e_t_h_o_d
_S_u_b_j_e_c_t_s. 49 four year olds (mean 4:11); 34 five year olds (mean 5:9);
23 seven year olds (mean 7:5); 27 nine year olds (mean 9:7). These children
were drawn from schools serving a predominantly middle class area in
southern England. Within each age group there were approximately equal
numbers of each sex.
_6
_M_a_t_e_r_i_a_l_s. 16 cards, each bearing a coloured line drawing of one of
the following objects: a car, a boat, a hat or a pair of trousers. Each type
of object was represented on four cards such that whilst the line drawing
remained the same on all four cards, two drawings were to a large scale, and
two to a small scale; one of the large scale drawings of each object was
coloured blue, the other red. One of the small scale drawings of each object
was yellow, the other green.
_P_r_o_c_e_d_u_r_e. Children were tested individually. The materials were
spread out, in random positions, on the table in front of the child. The
experimenter said: "You see these little cards here? In the game we're going
to play, I'm going to choose one. I'm going to choose it secretly, in my
head, and I'm not going to tell you which one I chose. The game is that you
have to find out which one I chose, by asking me questions. But here's the
tricky bit! I'm not allowed to say anything but 'yes' or 'no' to answer your
questions! Only 'yes' or 'no'. So you have to think of some good questions
to ask me, to find out which card I've chosen"
Since it is sometimes difficult to know exactly what a child's question
means from the form of the words alone, the children were asked to turn face
down, after each question, any cards which could now be eliminated. This
allows the experimenter to identify the information which the child actually
took from the reply to the question, rather than simply guessing from the
form of the words alone.
Although the child was led to believe that the target item had been
chosen in advance, this was not in fact the case. The experimenter always
responded so as to maximize the remaining search, but with the constraint
that each response be compatible with all previous responses.
Each child was asked to play this game three times, in a session
lasting between five and ten minutes. This procedure is substantially the
same as that reported by Thornton (1982).
7
_R_e_s_u_l_t_s _A_n_d _D_i_s_c_u_s_s_i_o_n.
As predicted, the incidence of within-session change in this task was
strongly associated with a subset of the strategies which children used in
addressing the problem:
Overall, five different strategies for addressing the Twenty Questions
problem were observed in this study. (All observed responses fell into one
of these five categories. Scorings were made by two independent judges,
yielding initial agreement of 93%. Discrepancies were the result of
carelessness and were quickly resolved). The five strategies observed here
were:
(1) Touching an individual object, with or without a demonstrative
expression such as: "That one?", and eliminating only that object after the
experimenter's response.
(2) Naming an individual object: i.e., identifying it by its basic
category name (e.g., "car?" or "the car?"). The child normally also touched
or pointed at a single card. They removed only one of the four cars on
receiving the experimenter's reply.
(3) Using an adjective alone to identify an individual object, for
example asking: "Is it red ?" or "Red ?". The child using this strategy
normally also touched or pointed at a single card. Despite its linguistic
peculiarity, children scored as using this last strategy clearly intended
only one object as their referent - in addition to touching or pointing at
one card, they would remove only one of several possible red items, after
asking "red ?", and would often ask the same question again later in the
game.
(4) Describing an individual object more fully, using both name and
adjective: for example asking: "the red car ?", and removing the red car
after the experimenter's response.
(5) Referring to a group of objects with a single question, and
8
removing the whole group in response to the experimenter's reply.
Patterns of usage of these strategies across different age groups are
shown in Table 1.
-------------------------
Insert Table 1 about here
-------------------------
None of the children who began the session asking about a group of
objects ever changed strategy in this study. By contrast, 58% of the
children who had begun by using one or other strategy for identifying single
objects subsequently changed their strategy. (This percentage was remarkably
similar at all three ages: 59% for four year olds, 55% for five year olds,
and 58% for seven year olds. Too few nine year olds asked about individual
objects to be considered here). This difference is relatively uninteresting,
in that the absence of change away from asking about groups of objects
probably reflects no more than that this strategy was the most
sophisticated; moves to a less sophisticated strategy would not be expected.
The analyzes of the relationship between change and specific strategies
below therefore concentrates on those children who began by identifying
individual objects, and omits those who started by asking about a group of
objects. This effectively omits the nine year old group from the analysis,
as there were too few to provide an interpretable sample.
Many children who changed their strategy during the experiment did so
more than once. The mean number of strategy changes observed per child
(among those showing a change) was 3.4, and the median 2. Whilst some
children (32%) who changed their strategy more than once used three or more
different strategies during the session, the majority used only two
strategies, moving from one to the other and then back again, etc. Analyses
below focus on initial strategy changes. Subsequent changes are hard to
interpret, since they are almost certainly not independent of the initial
9
change.
Among children asking about each object individually, there are
striking patterns of association between specific strategies and the
incidence of change. The data are summarized in Table 2. Collapsing data
across age shows that children naming objects more often changed strategy
than those touching them (chi square = 26.45, df = 1, N = 59, p<0.001).
Children naming objects also changed their strategy more often than those
describing objects more fully (chi square = 26.65, df = 1, N = 32, p<0.001).
Equally, children using adjectives alone more often changed strategy than
those describing the object more fully (Fisher exact test, p<0.01) or
touching it (chi square = 4.55, df = 1, N = 56, p<0.05).
These strategy effects are very strong. 100% of children who began the
task asking about individual objects by either by name or by adjective alone
changed their strategy at some point in the session. Only 26% of those
touching individual objects, and 17% of those describing objects more fully
ever changed strategy.
These effects hold up at each age: seven year olds using the "touch"
strategy are less likely to change strategy than those using the "adjective
alone" strategy (Fisher exact, p<0.005). Five year olds using the "naming"
strategy are more likely to change strategy than those using the "touch"
strategy (Fisher exact p<0.005) or the "describe more fully" strategy
(Fisher exact, p<0.01). Four year olds using the "naming" strategy are more
likely to change strategy than those using 'touch" (chi square = 16.12, df =
1, N = 38, p<0.001).
-------------------------
Insert Table 2 about here
-------------------------
This differential association between certain strategies and within-
session change also holds up where the only transition considered is that
10
from asking about single objects to asking about groups of objects.
20% of children who began by asking about individual objects switched
to asking about groups of objects at some point during the session. 15% of
four year olds, 20% of five year olds and 30% of seven year olds initially
asking about single objects showed this effect. The age trend fails to
reach statistical significance. This transition occurred in all three
games. For some children it was permanent. For others, the effect was more
transient, the child moving back and forth between questions about
individual objects and questions about groups.
Among five and seven year olds beginning the task either touching
individual objects or describing them fully, _n_o_n_e ever switched to asking
about a group of items. By contrast, fully 50% of five year olds, and 57%
of seven year olds who began by either naming individual objects or using an
adjective alone made the transition to eliminating groups of objects with
each question at some point during the experimental session.
This "strategies" effect was not observed for the four year old group.
Among 4 year old children, 12% of those initially either touching individual
objects or describing them fully switched to asking about a group of items.
Only 14% of children initially naming individual objects made this
transition.
Statistical analyzes of these effects were made, combining the two
strategies highly associated with change (naming and using an adjective
alone) and the two less associated with change (touching, and describing
more fully). The interaction between the linear component of age and
initial strategy type was significant (chi Square = 5.3, df = 1, p<0.05).
Examination of the data suggests partitioning the 4 year olds from the older
children. An analysis of the remaining four cells, relating to the five
year olds and the seven year olds, produces a non-significant age effect
(chi square = 0.1) but a significant effect for initial strategy (chi square
11
= 14.4, df = 1, p<0.001). There is no significant interaction between these
two terms. Thus for each of the two older groups, there is a strong effect
of initial strategy on the probability of their making the transition from
asking about individual objects to asking about groups of objects. The size
of this effect does not differ between the 5 year old and the 7 year old
group.
On this basis, the 5 and 7 year old data was aggregated and compared to
that for the 4 year old group. Two comparisons were made, one for the
children who initially identified individual objects by touch or by both
name and adjective together, and the other for the children who began by
identifying objects by either name alone or adjective alone. Where the first
strategy identified objects by name alone or adjective alone, the 5 and 7
year olds show significantly more change to asking about groups of objects
than do the 4 year olds (chi square = 5.3, df = 1, p<0.05). Where the first
strategy identified single objects by touch or by both name and adjective
together, there was no significant difference between the aggregated older
groups and the 4 year olds (chi square = 3.0, df = 1, NS).
Taking the results in relation to the incidence of within-session
change together, these data suggest that the same specific strategies
(naming and using an adjective alone) are more highly associated with
within-session change at all ages - but that there is a developmental
change, between the four and five year old groups, in the form that the
change takes. Specifically, the older children are more likely to show an
effect parallel to the developmental effect than are the four year olds.
The lesser tendency for four year olds to switch from asking about
individual objects to asking about groups of objects can be interpreted in
the context of the developmental changes observed in initial strategy
choices (Table 1). Age related change in patterns of strategy usage were
analyzed using the statistical package GLIM (Payne, 1986), and following the
12
procedure described by Fienberg (1980). This form of Chi Square allows the
decomposition of effects into main effects and interactions similar to that
obtained for continuous data with the use of analysis of variance.
As has been reported in other studies, the tendency to eliminate a
group of objects with a single question increased linearly with age (chi
square = 36.89, df = 1, p<0.001). This was the modal strategy used by the
nine year old group.
Analyses of the patterns of strategy choice among children asking about
individual objects exclude the nine year old group - since only two nine
year olds used a strategy of this type. For the remaining children, at the
start of the session there was a linear decline with age in the tendency to
make identifications by touch (chi square = 3.86, df = 1, p<0.05). The
tendency to identify individual objects by name showed a quadratic
relationship with age (chi square = 4.06, df = 1, p<0.05) - rising in
frequency between the four and five year old group, and thereafter falling.
There is no linear effect. Identification of an individual object by an
adjective alone showed a strong linear increase with age (chi square =
24.49, df = 1, p<0.001), contributed by the fact that only the oldest group
included in the analysis (the seven year olds) ever used it. There was no
significant linear relationship between age and the tendency to give fuller
descriptions of individual objects, but there is a quadratic effect (chi
square = 4.21, df = 1, p<0.05). 20% of five year olds asking about
individual objects began the task this way, in contrast to only 2.56% of
four year olds, and 8% of seven year olds.
These developmental changes in strategy can be largely characterized as
reflecting an age related increase in the child's success in handling the
variance present in a set of items (cf. Denney, Denney, & Ziobrowski, 1973;
Inhelder & Piaget, 1964; Kofsky, 1966; Thornton, 1982). The most
sophisticated use of the variance in the set is made in the strategy of
13
grouping items together, and eliminating a whole group with each question.
Strategies for describing individual objects, either fully or partially,
make less sophisticated use of the variance than this - but make more
explicit reference to it than is the case for the strategy of identifying
individual objects by touch alone. That the "touching" strategy is
associated with poorer use of the variance than the "naming" strategy (for
example) is confirmed by an analysis of successive item choices made by
children using each strategy: successive item choices can be scored 1 where
item N+1 is from the same category as item N, and 0 where it is not. Scoring
the first five choices from each game provides a score between 0 and 12.
Chance rates for these scores were approximated by 10,000 Monte Carlo runs
of artificial, computer generated subjects (this procedure corrects for the
non-independence of successive choices). Scores on this measure made by
children using the "naming" strategy differed from chance, both for the four
year old group (p<0.02) and for the five year old group (p<0.014), implying
that these children were implicitly grouping the items across successive
questions. By contrast, scores on this measure did not differ from chance,
at either four or five years, for children using the "touching" strategy,
implying that these children did not make implicit grouping across
successive questions. (Seven year olds were not included in this analysis
as few used either "touching" or "naming").
Thus age related patterns of initial strategy choice reflect differing
levels of usage of the variance in the set of items to be considered - with
four year old children showing very little use of variance. In this context,
one may infer that factors associated with skill in handling variance
limited the four year olds' capacity to show the transition from asking
about individual objects to asking about a group of objects, relative to
that of the older children.
The general thesis that age related change in the capacity for handling
14
the variance present in the set of objects constrains the direction or range
of change in children's procedures can be further explored by detailed
analysis of the overall direction of all types of change observed here. This
analysis supports the view that the four year olds are less successful than
their elders in moving to strategies using the variance in the set more
effectively. Table 3 summarizes the patterns of initial and final change
observed for children starting out either naming individual objects (four
and five year olds) or using adjectives alone (seven year olds). All of the
children change strategy. Few (only 13%) reverted to their original
strategy by the end of the game. 50% of four year olds had switched to
strategies making more explicit use of the variance by the end of the
session (i.e., describing more fully or asking about groups of objects), in
contrast to 80% of five year olds and 80% of seven year olds. Combining the
two older groups who initially named individual objects or used adjectives
alone, there is a significant tendency for both initial changes in strategy
(p = 0.006, Binomial) and final strategy choice (p = 0.018, Binomial) to be
toward more explicit use of the variance. Neither effect reaches
significance for the four year olds.
-------------------------
Insert Table 3 about here
-------------------------
Unlike the older groups, a fair number (8, i.e., 33%) of four year olds
initially touching individual objects also changed their strategies during
the session. Inevitably, any change away from touching must involve a
greater use of the variance, and so initial changes are of relatively little
interest in this context. Seven of the eight children initially switched
from touch to naming; of these, three subsequently tried describing items
more fully, and one tried - transiently - asking about a group of objects.
However, seven of the eight children reverted to touching individual objects
15
by the end of the session. This tendency to revert to the original strategy
is significantly greater for these children than for those starting out
naming individual objects or using adjectives alone (Chi square = 17.5 , df
= 1, p<0.001, N = 39). The remaining child who had begun by touching the
items ended the session describing items more fully. These data again
confirm the general view that the four year olds have some difficulty in
moving to the strategies involving better use of variance.
_C_o_n_c_l_u_s_i_o_n
The results obtained in this experiment confirm the prediction that
some strategies are more highly associated with the incidence of change than
others. The effect occurs for each age group examined. The principle
contribution of these strategies seems to be in triggering change: the
direction which change subsequently takes is a function of other factors, -
specifically here the child's capacity to handle the variance present in the
set of objects to be considered.
_E_x_p_e_r_i_m_e_n_t _2
The aim of this second study is to analyze the origins of the
"strategy" effect obtained above, specifically exploring the contributions
of semantic or syntactic factors.
In analyzing the genesis of any effect, it is as well to begin from a
consideration of the contribution made by the direct demands of the task.
The Twenty Questions task as used here was designed to be very open ended,
(as was described above). There is no task demand to be quicker, or more
efficient, or more sophisticated. (Only a minority of the observed phenomena
of change would meet these contingencies, in any case). Thus the observed
effects are not the result of contingencies created by the task per se. The
two strategies associated with change successfully satisfy the task demands
as much as do the strategies not associated with change.
One possible cavil to this last conclusion is immediately apparent:
16
from the point of view of an objective observer, the strategies highly
associated with change include a verbal label which partially - but only
partially - identifies the object the child intends to select. For example,
saying "car" in the "naming" strategy provides a label which narrows the
field of reference, but does not uniquely identify a single card, since
there are several cars in the set. There is, therefore, a certain ambiguity
about this label. Labels used in strategies which were not associated with
change were not ambiguous in this way: the "full description" strategy
provides a unique verbal identification of each item ("The red car", for
example). The "touching" strategy does not offer any verbal identification
of the object at all, but only a neutral reference in support of touch
("That one?").
One might hypothesize, therefore, that the ambiguity of reference
inherent in the "naming" and "adjective alone" strategies triggered change
by creating difficulties in communication, and hence generating a task
demand for change. But this is not the case. It should be recalled that
children using the "naming" and the "adjective alone" strategies virtually
always touched each object as they asked about it. Their questions were in
fact, therefore, no more ambiguous than those asked by children using the
"touching" strategy. There was no communicative difficulty associated with
these strategies at all.
Nor did the experimenter call attention to the potential ambiguity of a
verbal label: her "script" was to behave as if every question were clear in
every respect. A truly ambiguous question "car?" without clarificatory
pointing would have been answered straightforwardly (e.g., "no!"), and the
child left to interpret that reply as referring to any individual car - or
all of the cars - as he or she pleased. The experimenter always accepted
the child's interpretation (as revealed by subsequent behaviour) of her
responses as correct, without comment. In sum, this task does not present
17
either task demands or social cues suggesting a need for better or less
ambiguous communication.
Nevertheless, the inclusion of a potentially ambiguous label for
identifying an object might still have been the key factor leading the
"naming" and the "adjective alone" strategies to trigger change. A
fundamental rule in most communicative situations is to avoid ambiguity
(Grice, 1975). Rules of this kind are commonly over-extended, becoming ends
in themselves, rather than remaining subservient to the direct contingencies
of the task in hand (cf. Fodor, 1987; Piaget, 1977). The general rule to
avoid _a_c_t_u_a_l ambiguities may have been over-extended here, to include the
avoidance of even the _p_o_t_e_n_t_i_a_l ambiguities inherent in the inclusion of
ambiguous elements (labels) in a question.
The rule "avoid ambiguities" is a semantic rule, deriving from the
domain of communication, and predicating a direct manipulation of the
semantic content of a strategy in relation to its context. Thus the account
offered above of the strategy effects observed here implies that the
processes constraining the incidence of change in this task were primarily
domain-specific, semantic ones.
This hypothesis can be tested by altering the relationship between the
materials and the two strategies associated with change. The argument is
that "naming" and "adjective alone" strategies triggered change because the
child over-extended the rule "avoid ambiguity". These two strategies
trigger this rule only because there were several items sharing the labels
favoured by each strategy. If the materials could be changed so that there
was only one item corresponding to labels of these types, then the "naming"
and "adjective alone" strategies would not generate ambiguous labels, would
not trigger the rule, and would not be highly associated with the incidence
of change.
The aim of this second experiment is to test this hypothesis. The
18
specific design of the new set of materials to test the hypothesis will be
different, in relation to the "naming" strategy and the "adjective alone"
strategy. For simplicity, the study below explores the issue in relation to
only one of these strategies: the "naming" strategy.
In this second experiment, the set of items the child was asked to
consider fell into superordinate categories (e.g., "vehicles"), and included
only one item of each of the basic category labels (e.g., "car") favoured by
children using the "naming" strategy. Subjects were five year old children,
i.e., the age group most often using this strategy in Experiment 1. If the
association between the "naming" strategy and within-session change is
indeed driven by the over-extension of a rule specifying the avoidance of
ambiguity, then a comparison of results from Experiments 1 and 2 should show
that: (a) this association is much stronger in Experiment 1 than Experiment
2; (b) the incidence of within session change is much higher among children
using the "naming" strategy in Experiment 1 than for those using it in
Experiment 2.
In comparing the results from these two experiments, one would also
expect to see other differences, following on from factors identified by
Rosch (Rosch, Mervis & Boyes-Bream, 1976; Rosch, 1978). Rosch has shown
that young children will use classes at the level of basic categories much
more readily than classes at the level of superordinate categories, in a
range of situations. On this basis, one would expect fewer children in
Experiment 2 to begin the session asking about groups of objects, and fewer
to switch to this strategy during the experimental session than was observed
in Experiment 1.
The factors identified by Rosch are independent of those put forward
here to explain the incidence of within-session change. However, Rosch's
work must be taken into account in testing the predictions generated by the
"ambiguity" hypothesis: the first prediction from the "ambiguity" hypothesis
19
(that the degree of association between the "naming" strategy and change
will be different between Experiment 1 and 2) cannot be predicted on the
basis of the factors identified by Rosch. But the second "ambiguity"
hypothesis (that rates of within-session change will be greater in
Experiment 1 than in Experiment 2) also follows to some extent from Rosch's
work.
This difficulty in generating clear predictions from the "ambiguity"
hypothesis can, however, be easily overcome. Rosch's work would predict
only that the rate of change from asking about individual objects to asking
about groups will be lower in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1. Unlike the
"ambiguity" hypothesis, Rosch's work makes no predictions as to the relative
rates of change between one strategy for asking about individual objects and
another. Confounding effects from the factors identified by Rosch can
therefore be avoided by comparing specifically the rates of change between
one strategy for identifying individual objects and another, in testing the
"ambiguity" hypothesis.
Rosch's work on children's category use identifies a further area which
might be explored in comparing the results of Experiments 1 and 2. >From
that work, one would expect five year old children asking about individual
objects to show a strong preference for using the "naming" strategy rather
than other strategies for identifying individual objects - as was observed
in Experiment 1. Will this bias be still stronger in Experiment 2? If so,
then one may conclude that the same factors influencing children to abandon
naming within-session were also influential in determining initial strategy
choice.
In the analyzes below, the key comparisons in testing the "ambiguity"
hypothesis involve only those children who begin the task asking about
individual objects. Five year olds in Experiment 1 who began by asking
about a group of objects do not contribute to these analyzes. These
20
children were run (as a separate group) in Experiment 2, in addition to the
sample of new, naive subjects. The inclusion of this "experienced" sample
in Experiment 2 adds power to the test of the hypothesis of interest here:
if these children behave as predicted with the superordinate materials
(i.e., identifying individual objects by name, showing low rates of
within-session change overall, and no association between "naming" and
change) then one may be confident that this reflects factors governing
strategy choices, and not simply an inability to use, or to think of,
alternative strategies.
_M_e_t_h_o_d
_S_u_b_j_e_c_t_s.
37 five year olds (mean age 5 years 8 months). 19 children (the
"naive" group) were new to the task. 18 children (the "experienced" group)
had all done the task before, with the materials used in Experiment 1, and
had all asked about groups of objects from the outset. 14 of these were
subjects from Experiment 1, and 4 were children from a pilot study, added to
bring up the N in this group.
_M_a_t_e_r_i_a_l_s.
16 objects, each drawn on a separate card as in Experiment 1. The
objects fall into four superordinate categories (transports, furniture,
fruit, clothing). The drawings were coloured: 4 were yellow (boat, chair,
banana, hat). 4 were red (trousers, television set, train, cherries). 4
were green (bed, plane, shirt, apple). 3 were blue (car, table, glove).
The orange was orange.
_P_r_o_c_e_d_u_r_e.
The procedure and instructions were as in Experiment 1. Children in the
"experienced" group were tested shortly after completing the task used in
experiment one. Results obtained for the two new groups of children were
compared to those obtained for five year olds in Experiment 1.
21
_R_e_s_u_l_t_s _a_n_d _d_i_s_c_u_s_s_i_o_n
The predictions derived from the hypothesis that "naming" triggered
change in Experiment 1 through the overextension of a rule to avoid
ambiguities were supported in the results from this experiment.
The key comparisons in testing the "ambiguity" hypothesis involved only
those children who had begun the session asking about individual objects.
20 five year olds from Experiment 1 (i.e., 59%) took part in this analysis,
as did 34 five year olds (i.e., 92%) from Experiment 2. (Conflating "naive"
and "experienced" children asking about each object individually in
Experiment 2 in this sample does not confound within and between subject
variance, in comparing this group with subjects from Experiment 1: all
"experienced" subjects had asked about groups in Experiment 1, and so were
excluded from the Experiment 1 sample used here).
As these figures imply, (and confirming expectations derived from
Rosch's work) the tendency to ask about groups of objects was significantly
greater in Experiment 1 than in Experiment 2. Whereas 41% of the five year
olds in Experiment 1 initially asked about groups of objects, none of the
naive five year olds in Experiment 2 did so (chi square = 10.6, df = 1, N =
53 p<0.01). More dramatically, of the 18 "experienced" subjects, all of whom
had begun by asking about groups of objects in Experiment 1, only 3 did so
in Experiment 2. The difference between the two conditions is significant
(Mcnemar test, chi square = 17, df = 1, p<0.001). This last result confirms
Rosch's thesis that children's use of categories in problem-solving reflects
the structure of the categories into which materials fall: the effect
observed here cannot be explained as a general inability to use groups, or a
general metacognitive failure to realize the utility/possibility of doing
so: 15 of 18 children failing to use groups here had done so, with different
material, shortly beforehand.
The prediction that the incidence of change will be more highly
22
associated with the naming strategy in Experiment 1 than in Experiment 2 was
supported. Whereas in Experiment 1, 10 of the 11 children showing within-
session change (91%) originally used the naming strategy, none of those
showing within-session change in Experiment 2 used this strategy. The
difference is significant (fisher exact, p = 0.025). Thus the association
between the naming strategy and a high incidence of within session change in
Experiment 1 was a function of the fit between that strategy and the
materials.
The prediction that there would be a lower overall rate of within-
session change between one strategy for identifying individual objects and
another in Experiment 2 as compared with Experiment 1 was also supported.
The analysis omits all children who subsequently switched to asking about
groups of objects (four in Experiment 1, none in Experiment 2). Of the
residual children, seven of the 16 from Experiment 1 (i.e., 44%) switched
between one strategy for identifying single objects and another, whereas
only three of the 34 children from Experiment 2 (i.e., 9%) did so. The
difference is significant (Chi square = 8.3, df = 1, N = 50, p<0.01).
Among children asking about individual objects, 80% of those in
Experiment 2 began by naming items, as opposed to only 50% who did so in
Experiment 1. The difference is significant (chi square = 6.31, df = 1, N =
54, p<0.02).
_C_o_n_c_l_u_s_i_o_n
The results of this second experiment fully support the conclusion that
the "naming" strategy triggered within-session change in Experiment 1
through the overextension of a rule specifying the avoidance of ambiguities.
With materials which are uniquely described by the labels favoured in this
strategy, the association between naming and change disappears, and the
overall incidence of change declines. Initial strategy choice is also
constrained by this factor: the tendency to start the session using the
23
naming strategy is significantly greater where that strategy provides unique
reference than where it provides potentially ambiguous reference.
_G_e_n_e_r_a_l _D_i_s_c_u_s_s_i_o_n
The studies reported here support the thesis that within-session change
is more highly associated with some strategies than with others. This
relationship is in fact predicted by virtually all existing models of
mechanisms of cognitive change - but it is seldom explicitly considered, or
taken into account in theoretical models. It is worth pointing out that
differential strategy effects can easily be overlooked, unless explicitly
sought out: e.g., Thornton's (1982) analysis assumed (but did not test for)
equal association between strategies and change in the Twenty Questions
task, and drew theoretical inferences accordingly. The results obtained
here demonstrate the utility of explicit analysis of strategy effects in
constraining models of change. More than this, the demonstration of so
strong a strategy effect here must call into question the interpretation of
any micro-analytic study of the genesis of within-session change which does
not explore, and take account of possible strategy effects.
In the within-session phenomena observed here, strategy effects
interact with other factors in determining change in children's problem-
solving. For instance, the same strategy (naming individual objects) is the
principle origin of within-session change for both four and five year old
children. But the five year old group are more likely to change in the
direction of strategies making more explicit use of the variance in the set
of items (describing fully, asking about groups) than are the four year
olds. In effect, the strategy seems to trigger change, and then other
factors constrain the direction which that change takes (here, these other
factors are presumably connected with well documented developmental change
in children's representations of classifications and variance - cf. Denney
24
et al., 1973; Kofsky, 1966; Thornton, 1982). The result endorses the view
that studies of within-session change may tap only a subset of the factors
contributing to developmental effects.
The data support the conclusion that the within-session effects
observed in Experiment 1 were due to the application of a rule specifying
the avoidance of even potential ambiguities. This rule is semantic,
domain-specific: it derives from the domain of communication, and is
moderated by the detailed environment in which it is used. The implication
is that semantic, domain-specific processes, rather than syntactic domain
general ones played the critical role in the incidence of change in this
task.
No doubt it would be possible to adduce a syntactic model which would
account for the data equally well. No empirical analysis of a mental
process can ever be definitive. However, a syntactic account of these data
would be post hoc. No presently published mechanisms would predict it. The
proposed semantic rule provides a better hypothesis, in that it actually
predicted the observed effects in a principled way. At the very least, it
must be taken into account as an active factor in these phenomena, until
directly discredited.
Nevertheless, the conclusion that semantic, domain-specific processes
played a major role in determining the incidence of change in this task does
not necessarily imply that syntactic, domain general ones are not also
important. Rather, what it does is to place constraints on the phenomena to
be explained through syntactic models. For example, one might argue here
that semantic processes are responsible for triggering, and for setting up
the broad goals for change. However, it is possible, and even likely, that
syntactic processes contribute to the actual generation and shape of the
ensuing change.
Of course, the identification of semantic factors as the principle
25
determinants of change in this task does not allow the conclusion that this
will be the case in every task. That is an empirical question, requiring
the analysis of many different tasks. But it is the nature of detailed
research of this kind to progress one task at a time. The occurrence of
such an effect here demonstrates that semantic processes can contribute
importantly to change, and shows that the identification of such effects can
constrain inferences as to the functioning of syntactic elements. It
emphasizes the need for semantic processes to be explored more directly in
other situations, both to understand them for themselves, and to focus
studies of syntactic effects. More detailed studies of the interaction
between specific strategies and cognitive changes can provide rich
constraints on models of change, and an arena for the exploration of the
interactions between semantic, domain-specific and syntactic, domain general
ones.
26
References
Anderson, J. R., (1983). _T_h_e _a_r_c_h_i_t_e_c_t_u_r_e _o_f _c_o_g_n_i_t_i_o_n. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Carey, S., (1985). _C_o_n_c_e_p_t_u_a_l _C_h_a_n_g_e _i_n _C_h_i_l_d_h_o_o_d. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Denney, J. R., Denney, N. W., & Ziobrowski, M. J. (1973). Alterations in
the information processing strategies of young children following
observation of adult models. _D_e_v_e_l_o_p_m_e_n_t_a_l _P_s_y_c_h_o_l_o_g_y, _8, 202-208.
Fienberg, S. E. (1980). _T_h_e _a_n_a_l_y_s_i_s _o_f _c_r_o_s_s-_c_l_a_s_s_i_f_i_e_d _c_a_t_e_g_o_r_i_c_a_l _d_a_t_a.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. A. (1987). _P_s_y_c_h_o_s_e_m_a_n_t_i_c_s: _T_h_e _p_r_o_b_l_e_m _o_f _m_e_a_n_i_n_g _i_n _t_h_e
_p_h_i_l_o_s_o_p_h_y _o_f _m_i_n_d. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan
(Eds.), _S_p_e_e_c_h _a_c_t_s. New York: Academic Press.
Hunt, E. (1989). Cognitive science: Definition, status and question.
_A_n_n_u_a_l _R_e_v_i_e_w _o_f _P_s_y_c_h_o_l_o_g_y, _4_0, 603-629.
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1964). _T_h_e _e_a_r_l_y _g_r_o_w_t_h _o_f _l_o_g_i_c _i_n _t_h_e _c_h_i_l_d.
New York: Harper & Row.
Inhelder, B., Sinclair, H., & Bovet, M. (1974). _L_e_a_r_n_i_n_g _a_n_d _t_h_e
_d_e_v_e_l_o_p_m_e_n_t _o_f _c_o_g_n_i_t_i_o_n. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). Micro and macro developmental changes in
language acquisition and other representational systems. _C_o_g_n_i_t_i_v_e
_S_c_i_e_n_c_e, _3, 81-118.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1984). Children's problem solving. In M. E. Lamb, A.
L. Brown, & B. Rogoff (Eds.), _A_d_v_a_n_c_e_s _i_n _d_e_v_e_l_o_p_m_e_n_t_a_l _p_s_y_c_h_o_l_o_g_y Vol.
3). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). _B_e_y_o_n_d _m_o_d_u_l_a_r_i_t_y. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Keil, F. (1984). Mechanisms in cognitive development and the structure of
knowledge. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), _M_e_c_h_a_n_i_s_m_s _o_f _c_o_g_n_i_t_i_v_e _d_e_v_e_l_o_p_m_e_n_t.
27
New York: Freeman
Keil, F. (1986). On the structure-dependent nature of stages of cognitive
development. In I. Levin (Ed.), _S_t_a_g_e _a_n_d _s_t_r_u_c_t_u_r_e: _R_e_o_p_e_n_i_n_g _t_h_e
_d_e_b_a_t_e.
Klahr, D. (1982). Non-monotone assessment of monotone development in
information processing analysis. In S. Strauss & R. Stavey (Eds.), _U-
_s_h_a_p_e_d _b_e_h_a_v_i_o_r_a_l _g_r_o_w_t_h. New York: Academic Press.
Klahr, D. (1984). Transition processes in quantitative development. In R.
Sternberg (Ed.), _M_e_c_h_a_n_i_s_m_s _o_f _c_o_g_n_i_t_i_v_e _d_e_v_e_l_o_p_m_e_n_t. New York: Freeman.
Kofsky, E. (1966). A scalogram study of classificatory development. _C_h_i_l_d
_D_e_v_e_l_o_p_m_e_n_t, _3_7, 191-204.
Mosher, F. A., & Hornsby, J. R. (1966). On asking questions. In J. Bruner
et al. (Eds.), _S_t_u_d_i_e_s _i_n _c_o_g_n_i_t_i_v_e _g_r_o_w_t_h. New York: Wiley.
Payne, C. D. (Ed.). (1986). _T_h_e _G_L_I_M _s_y_s_t_e_m. Royal Statistical Society.
Piaget, J. (1976). In W. Overton & J. Gallagher (Eds.), _K_n_o_w_l_e_d_g_e _a_n_d
_d_e_v_e_l_o_p_m_e_n_t: _V_o_l. _1. _A_d_v_a_n_c_e_s _i_n _r_e_s_e_a_r_c_h _a_n_d _t_h_e_o_r_y. New York: Plenum
Press.
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd
(Eds.), _C_o_g_n_i_t_i_o_n _a_n_d _c_a_t_e_g_o_r_i_s_a_t_i_o_n. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., & Boyes-Beam, P. (1976). Basic
objects in natural categories. _C_o_g_n_i_t_i_v_e _P_s_y_c_h_o_l_o_g_y, _8, 382-439.
Siegler, R. S. & Jenkins, E. (1989). _H_o_w _c_h_i_l_d_r_e_n _d_i_s_c_o_v_e_r _n_e_w _s_t_r_a_t_e_g_i_e_s.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Thornton, S. (1982). Challenging "Early Competence": a process-oriented
analysis of children's classifying. _C_o_g_n_i_t_i_v_e _S_c_i_e_n_c_e, _6, 77-100.
Wales, R. & Thornton, S. (1991). What's new? Psychological issues for
modelling creativity. IJCAI satellite conference, Queensland. (and
forthcoming).
28
_T_A_B_L_E _1
_N_u_m_b_e_r _o_f _C_h_i_l_d_r_e_n _a_t _E_a_c_h _A_g_e _U_s_i_n_g _E_a_c_h _o_f _t_h_e _5 _S_t_r_a_t_e_g_i_e_s _f_o_r
_I_d_e_n_t_i_f_y_i_n_g _O_b_j_e_c_t_s _i_n _t_h_e _2_0 _Q_u_e_s_t_i_o_n_s _G_a_m_e
________________________________________________________________
Four Five Seven Nine
Years Years Years Years
________________________________________________________________
Touching 24 6 4 2
Naming 14 10 1 0
Adjective alone 0 0 6 0
Describing fully 1 4 1 0
Eliminating a 10 14 11 25
Group of Objects
________________________________________________________________
29
_T_A_B_L_E _2
_P_e_r_c_e_n_t_a_g_e_s _o_f _C_h_i_l_d_r_e_n _W_h_o _E_v_e_r _C_h_a_n_g_e_d _S_t_r_a_t_e_g_y, _a_s _a _F_u_n_c_t_i_o_n
_o_f _A_g_e _a_n_d _I_n_i_t_i_a_l _S_t_r_a_t_e_g_y
__________________________________________________________________
Original Four Five Seven Overall
Strategy Years Years Years Mean
__________________________________________________________________
Touching 33.3% 16.6% 0% 26%
Naming 100% 100% 100% 100%
Adjective Alone NA NA 100% 100%
Describing Fully 100%* 0% 0% 17%
*(N=1 child)
__________________________________________________________________
30
_T_A_B_L_E _3
_S_e_c_o_n_d _a_n_d _F_i_n_a_l _C_h_o_i_c_e _o_f _S_t_r_a_t_e_g_y _M_a_d_e _b_y _C_h_i_l_d_r_e_n _S_t_a_r_t_i_n_g _b_y
_I_d_e_n_t_i_f_y_i_n_g _I_n_d_i_v_i_d_u_a_l _O_b_j_e_c_t_s _E_i_t_h_e_r _b_y _N_a_m_e _o_r _b_y _U_s_i_n_g _a_n
_A_d_j_e_c_t_i_v_e _A_l_o_n_e - (_P_e_r_c_e_n_t_a_g_e_s _b_y _A_g_e)
____________________________________________________________________
Second Strategy Chosen (i.e., First Change)
touching describing eliminating
fully a group
____________________________________________________________________
4 Years 43% 43% 14%
(initial strategy
naming)
5 Years 30% 50% 20%
(initial strategy
naming)
7 Years 0% 33% 66%
(initial strategy
adjective alone)
____________________________________________________________________
____________________________________________________________________
Final Strategy Used
touching describing eliminating reverts to
fully a group 1st strategy
____________________________________________________________________
4 Years 43% 28% 14% 14%
(initial strategy
naming)
5 Years 20% 50% 30% 0%
(initial strategy
naming)
7 Years 14% 14% 43% 28%
(initial strategy
adjective alone)
____________________________________________________________________
31